Thursday
morning at work, I prepared to fetch our building’s mail and discovered
a crinkled sticky note in the drawer beside the mail key. Curious, I
picked it up and immediately recognized the handwriting. It was my
grocery list, which I had found in my coat pocket when I’d started to
get the mail the day before, and which I had thought I’d thrown away! I
shared my amusement with my coworker, tossed it as I’d intended, and
proceeded to carry a used printer cartridge and some outgoing letters to
the Student Union, planning as I walked to take the bulky cartridge box
down to the campus store first so I need not carry it farther than
necessary—yet I made it past the stairs and down the hall toward the
mailboxes before I realized I still had the cumbersome box in my arms!
These
are hardly isolated incidents of absentmindedness, nor circumstances
restricted to intellectual types like myself; I’ll wager everyone’s
nearly put the cereal in the fridge and the milk on the shelf, or
another such mix-up. While insufficient attention may be due to
sleepiness, illness, medication, or natural brain structure, the fault
in cases like mine is more often distraction with extraneous thoughts, sights, and sounds--or hyperfocus on one task to the detriment
others, including mindfulness of one's surroundings. I theorize (based on personal observation rather
than any more-comprehensive scientific method) that most of us go
through a majority of the day on autopilot so as to free our brains for
other ruminations: observations about the sparrows in the bushes, plans
for dinner, speculation about plot developments in last night’s tv show,
and of course, daydreams about saving the world (or a town, or a beau,
or maybe just a cat) from certain destruction. Now what was I
originally writing about? Oh, yes—absentmindedness.
Most
people can do two complementary activities competently, such as washing
dishes and talking, or listening and taking notes (though someone with a
learning disability may only be able to do one at a time), and in this
division of thought, one or the other may get shortchanged, resulting in
mental slips or “crossed wires.” For instance, we may eat more than we
intended because our minds have focused on what we’re reading or
watching, or we may write what we overhear someone say instead of what
our minds planned to write.
I
feel I must sidetrack here to make a distinction: Complementary
activities differ somewhat from multitasking, which is when a person
quickly alternates between multiple tasks that each requires
most that person’s cognition and attention. For example, one might be
scripting in a computer language while following the plot of a tv show
and maintaining an IM conversation and an occasional face-to-face
conversation. The complementary example of washing dishes and talking
truly can
be done simultaneously because one action—typically the physical
task—happens automatically, guided by muscle memory and subconscious
thought, while the other task takes up the bulk of the person’s
conscious thoughts. However, in this multitasking example, the brain
can only focus on one at a time and must restart and refocus on each
separate task—sometimes so quickly they seem simultaneous, but that is
not the case, and the brain may refocus incompletely so that, in the
end, all tasks take longer to complete and are prone to confusion and
error. This is why we’re advised not to text or talk on cell phones
when we drive—because fiddling with the phone uses the mental energy
that we expend to monitor our mental autopilot, thus decreasing our
powers of observation and our reaction time and, conversely, increasing
our chance of getting into accidents. Complementary activities are
likewise prone to error—particularly when our conscious mind stops
monitoring our autopiloted actions. Thus, mental abstraction from tasks
results in our autopilot driving us to work when we wanted to go to the
store or locking our keys in the house when we merely step out to check
the mail.
Abstraction
can certainly cause problems, but at certain times, it is a desirable
state of mind, particularly during life’s inevitable moments of tedium
such as may come during periods of waiting or repetition. Regarding the
latter, I will not here expound upon the benefits or detriments of
following a routine, but we all know that despite how our daily details
may vary, we do have
routines--places we frequent, habitual gestures, methods of completing
individual tasks from brushing teeth to shoveling snow, and so on.
Furthermore, our schedules tend to remain steady from day to day, week
to week, or year to year. Yet despite the human proclivity to develop
habits and our preference for constancy—so comforting in their stability
and familiarity—William Cowper said it well that “Variety’s the very
spice of life / that gives it all its flavor” (from “The Task”). The
mind craves this variety to feed its creativity and sate its desire for
mental stimulation to keep life from becoming stale to us. Thus, if
one’s routine and circumstances prevent much external diversity of occupation, the only remaining source of diversity is internal,
and this source is by no means deficient, for one can never claim to be
bored who has an intelligent, active mind; reason and imagination can
supply both occupation and entertainment during every long delay and
every tedious task. While sitting still or laboring on a simple and
familiar physical task, people can engage their mental autopilot,
freeing their minds to think or their mouths to talk as they act—except
for the small part of the conscious mind that observes one’s
surroundings as a safety monitor and draws the conscious mind’s
attention when something deviates from what it expects.
Thus, absentmindedness is fun and relatively harmless excursion from
reality--when permitted in a safe environment while the body performs
safe and relatively unimportant tasks--but when careless errors may lose
us our dignity, our safety, our job, or especially others’ lives, it’s
better to keep our minds on the task at hand.
No comments:
Post a Comment